Level | Grade | N |
---|---|---|
Primary | 6 | 30 |
Primary | 7 | 29 |
Primary | 8 | 26 |
Secondary | 1 | 29 |
Secondary | 2 | 20 |
Secondary | 3 | 22 |
To test the main hypothesis, whether passive parental consent is
not perceived as less appropriate than active parental consent, we used
a reversed inferior test. We defined a one-sided t-test with the
alternative hypothesis that active parental consent is perceived as more
appropriate than passive parental consent. However, to confirm our
hypothesis we were interested in the amount of evidence for the null
hypothesis, meaning we would accept our hypothesis when the Bayes factor
for the alternative hypothesis is smaller than 3 (BF10 < .33; BF01
> 3).
Participants were exposed to 10 video vignettes, each explaining one of the research types. Participants indicated how appropriate both active and passive parental informed consent is for each vignette. Participants could rate the appropriateness on a 7-point slider scale, ranging from not appropriate at all (1) to very appropriate (7). The ratings of active and passive consent were asked on two separate pages, and each time the illustration of the vignette was presented alongside the research method, see Figure 1. Table 2 and figure 3 present the mean ratings of passive and active consent, for the ten research types. In addition, the table reports the Bayes factors for the alternative hypotheses and the conclusions that we draw.
Research type | Mean | 95% CI | Mean | 95% CI | Bayes factor | Conclusion | Cohens D |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Observational | 5.06 | [4.77, 5.36] | 4.74 | [4.45, 5.02] | 0.04 | Pas = Act | 0.11 |
Co-creation | 4.69 | [4.41, 4.96] | 5.29 | [5.05, 5.53] | 0.40 | Pas = Act | 0.11 |
Survey | 4.13 | [3.82, 4.45] | 5.51 | [5.26, 5.76] | 10.44 | Pas < Act | 0.23 |
Longitudinal Survey | 3.58 | [3.27, 3.89] | 5.42 | [5.17, 5.68] | 27.40 | Pas < Act | 0.26 |
Focus group | 4.84 | [4.55, 5.13] | 5.13 | [4.88, 5.39] | >100 | Pas < Act | 0.34 |
Interview | 4.48 | [4.18, 4.78] | 5.16 | [4.91, 5.41] | >100 | Pas < Act | 0.49 |
Objective Measures | 3.48 | [3.17, 3.79] | 5.50 | [5.25, 5.75] | >100 | Pas < Act | 0.66 |
Diary | 4.50 | [4.20, 4.80] | 5.42 | [5.18, 5.66] | >100 | Pas < Act | 0.71 |
Data Donation | 3.54 | [3.24, 3.85] | 5.46 | [5.21, 5.70] | >100 | Pas < Act | 0.73 |
Clinical | 2.72 | [2.41, 3.04] | 5.51 | [5.20, 5.81] | >100 | Pas < Act | 0.92 |
Because we were interested in potential differences between
primary school and secondary school children, we stratified our analyses
by the school level. A Bayesian mixed-effects model indicated strong
evidence that there is no difference in scores in general between
primary and secondary school (BF10 = .08 ± 1.02%). This means that
overall (irrespective of the consent procedure or research type),
parents rated the acceptability similarly. However, we found anecdotal
evidence (BF10 = 2.88 ± 0.93%) that the differences between active and
passive consent were bigger in primary school (active = 5.54, passive =
3.91, difference = 1.54) than secondary school (active = 5.15, passive =
4.34, difference = 0.81).
Splitting the vignettes into primary and secondary schools provided the most interesting insights. Specifically, the same Bayesian paired samples t-tests from the preregistered analyses were run for the primary and secondary schools separately. For the primary school children, no different conclusions were drawn. For children in secondary school (n = 71), we found anecdotal to moderate evidence that parents do not think that active consent is more appropriate for Focus groups (BF01 = 2.63 ± 0.00%), Survey (BF01 = 2.08 ± 0.00%), and Longitudinal Survey (BF01 = 3.01 ± 0.00%) studies. This difference is also observable when comparing Figure 3 with Figure 4.
Based on these results we can tentatively state that for secondary schools, passive parental consent is an appropriate procedure for Observational, Co-creation, Survey, Longitudinal survey, and Focus group studies.
We were interested in whether parents would regard certain
topics of interest to our projects as more or less appropriate for
passive consent. Therefore we asked the parents how appropriate passive
consent is for surveys when we ask questions pertaining to their
happiness and mental well-being, their
friends, the situation at home, and personally
identifiable information such as address or date of birth. In Table
3 and Figure 5, we have also added the general appropriateness of
passive consent from the vignettes. The general label refers to the
appropriateness of passive consent for surveys from the vignettes. As
indicated in the table and the figure, the scores for happiness
and friends were similar to the general sentiment of using
passive consent in survey studies. However, parents were a bit more
reserved about questions on the domestic situation and
especially about personally identifiable information.
Mean | SD | SE | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Happiness | 4.72 | 1.71 | 0.14 | [4.46, 4.99] |
Friend | 4.65 | 1.68 | 0.13 | [4.39, 4.92] |
Domestic | 4.17 | 2.01 | 0.16 | [3.85, 4.48] |
Personal | 3.60 | 2.12 | 0.17 | [3.26, 3.93] |
General | 4.69 | 1.76 | 0.14 | [4.41, 4.96] |
In our lab, we like to use a blended science approach. In our
projects, we cooperate with youth to design our studies. Specifically,
together with the children we co-create media content such as Instagram
posts or TikTok videos. Therefore, we asked the parents how appropriate
passive consent is for co-creation when participants will be making
photos or videos.
With a mean score of 3.57, this was not perceived as appropriate by the parents. This score is below the mid-point of the scale and considerably lower than the appropriateness of passive consent for co-creation in general (4.69). Therefore, we advise researchers to use active parental consent procedures when co-creating media messages. This is also in line with the previous reservation about personally identifiable information. Most media content could contain some form of information that is identifiable, such as names, places, or faces.
The parents in the study were shown a video about a specific
format for obtaining parental consent for multiple studies within one
school year. Specifically, parents are asked to provide active parental
consent at the start of the school year for a series of studies on a
determined topic. For each study within the school year, parents are
informed and have the opportunity to opt out. We have termed this
cluster consent. The parents in this study were asked to
respond to the idea of the cluster consent procedure in general. Again
they used the faces scale and could select the face that best matched
their beliefs. The range was between 1 and 7 as shown in Figure 1.
Compared to the average scores of the 10 vignettes, the cluster consent scored a bit higher. But, be aware that these two scores are not entirely comparable. The passive and active scores are the averages of the vignette. The cluster score was just one single item measuring appropriateness in general.
Mean | SD | SE | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Active | 5.31 | 1.66 | 0.13 | [5.05, 5.57] |
Passive | 4.10 | 2.04 | 0.16 | [3.78, 4.42] |
Cluster | 5.45 | 1.79 | 0.14 | [5.17, 5.73] |
In the next question, participants gave a bit more information
about the cluster consent for the different research methods. Contrary
to the vignettes, they did not rate the appropriateness on a scale from
1 to 7, but they could indicate per research type if they thought the
cluster consent would be appropriate (yes vs no). Here are the
percentages of “yes” per research method. As can be seen in Figure 6,
the majority of the parents was open to the concept of cluster consent
for Survey, Observation, Longitudinal survey,
Co-creation, Interviews, and Diary studies.
Much to our surprise, using cluster consent procedures for Focus
group studies was only accepted by a minority of the parents.
Participants were asked whether they have been approached
previously to provide consent for their child in academic research.
Follow-up questions asked parents how they responded or why they did not
respond previously.
As can be seen in Table 5, the majority of the parents in our sample was never approached before to provide consent for their child to participate in scientific research.
Previously approached | Active | Passive | Difference |
---|---|---|---|
No | 5.28 | 4.03 | 1.25 |
Yes | 5.50 | 4.22 | 1.28 |
Of the 37 participants that were approached previously, almost
everybody did respond! Only two people answered that they did not
respond to a previous invite, see Table 7. In these cases, participants
were provided a text box in which they could explain why they did not
respond when approached.
The first participant indicated that (s)he had forgotten to respond. The second participant said (s)he did not respond because a passive consent procedure was used. Based on only these two participants we can not learn a lot. But at least, out of those 37 parents, there were no critical opponents of the consent procedure that was used in that particular study.
Previously approached | Number of Participants | Percent |
---|---|---|
Yes | 37 | 0.24 |
No | 106 | 0.68 |
Not sure | 13 | 0.08 |
Given that so few of our participants were approached before, we
wondered if this had an effect on the ratings for active and passive
consent. We deemed it plausible that the previous invitation might
increase trust in scientific research, or might have activated more
critical thinking.
The ratings of both active and passive consent were slightly higher for those who have been approached previously, see Table 6. However, it is very unlikely that there is a difference in ratings between parents who have been approached before and parents who haven’t (BF = 0.21). Also, we find no differences specifically for active (BF = 0.28) and passive (BF = 0.21) consent, and the differences between active and passive consent were comparable between both groups (BF = 0.15). Therefore, we can not conclude that having previously been invited to provide consent for your child does affect the rating for appropriateness for either active of passive parental consent procedures.
Reponded | Number of Participants | Percent |
---|---|---|
Yes | 35 | 94.59 |
No | 2 | 5.41 |
Participants were asked via which channels they like to opt in
or opt out of the study. Participants could select multiple channels. In
the figure, we have plotted the percentage of participants that selected
the channel. we have annotated the exact number of participants next to
the end of the bar. Because participants could select multiple channels,
the total of the percentages does not add up to 100%.
As can be seen in Figure 7, Email and website were
the most selected channels. When participants selected other,
they could give a suggestion. One of these suggestions was via an
app, the other text field was left blank.
Moreover,
participants were asked how important it is that communication about the
study is done in the same way the school normally communicates with the
parents. The participants could respond with not at all (1) to
very important (7). On average, the score was 5.82 (SD = 1.28),
meaning that most of them agreed that it is important to use the
schools’ existing channels of communication, see Figure 8.
In short, we advise using the schools’ existing channels, preferably email or website, for the consent procedure.
Participants were asked what they think would be a fair reward
for participation in scientific studies. Participants could select
multiple rewards. In the figure, we have plotted the percentage of
participants that selected the channel. we have annotated the exact
number of participants next to the end of the bar. Because participants
could select multiple rewards, the total of the percentages does not add
up to 100%.
As can be seen in Figure 10, a gift for the whole class and a gift for the individual child were the most selected rewards. Around one in three participants indicated that it was also not necessary to reward the participants, as it is important to participate in scientific studies. When participants selected other, they could give a suggestion.
One of these suggestions was to donate money to charity chosen by the children. Another suggestion was to hand out discount coupons for an excursion. The last interesting suggestion was to let the children decide what they want as a reward.
Payment | N | Mean | Median | Mode | Min | Max |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Vouchers | 20 | 8.12 | 5 | 5 | 1 | 50 |
Wire money to child | 15 | 6.17 | 5 | 5 | 1 | 15 |
Wire money to parent | 2 | 10.00 | 10 | NA | 5 | 15 |
Pay in cash | 2 | 5.50 | 6 | NA | 1 | 10 |
In addition, participants who selected some form of momentary
reward were asked to add what they thought would be a fair amount of
euros per hour. As can be seen, the amount varies heavily, between 1€
and 50€ (!) per hour. But looking at the mean (7,33€), median, and mode
(both 5€), the results suggest that 5€ per hour is reasonable for the
parents.
Erasmus University Rotterdam (EUR) is an internationally oriented university with a strong social orientation in its education and research, as expressed in our mission ‘Creating positive societal impact’. EUR is home to 3.700 academics and professionals and almost 33.000 students from more than 140 countries. Everything we do, we do under the credo The Erasmian Way – Making Minds Matter. We’re global citizens, connecting, entrepreneurial, open-minded, and socially involved. These Erasmian Values function as our internal compass and create EUR’s distinctive and recognizable profile. From these values, with a broad perspective and with an eye for diversity, different backgrounds and opinions, our employees work closely together to solve societal challenges from the dynamic and cosmopolitan city of Rotterdam. Thanks to the high quality and positive societal impact of our research and education, EUR can compete with the top European universities.
Young people grow up in a world where computers, tablets and smartphones are omnipresent. These digital media technologies offer important opportunities in the fields of entertainment, education, communication and cultural development. This being said, there are also risks involved that can affect their well-being and safety. Our research highlights the ways in which young people can take full advantage of the opportunities that digital media provides. Simultaneously, we emphasize how youth can cope with the potential risks of digital media use. Education to promote media literacy is indispensable for this. MediaMovez helps in the development of effective media literacy education programs. Through research, we can contribute by developing education programs that help children interact with digital media in a more autonomous, safe and responsible way. MediaMovez uses a media empowerment approach. This approach aims to strengthen the skills that young people need to use digital media independently and in a media-smart way.
Visit our Linkedin page.
Have a look at our Website.
so get in contact via email directly: movez@eur.nl
SocialMovez
Online social networks such as Instagram, TikTok and Snapchat offer unprecedented technological possibilities to promote a healthy lifestyle among young people. For example, through influencers in their online network. With our research, we want to make online health campaigns more effective while safeguarding young people’s digital privacy. SocialMovez investigates how health campaigns for young people can be spread more effectively through peer influencers in online social networks. We focus on promoting healthy behaviors such as physical activity or healthy eating habits. In our research we aim to identify peer influencers in young people’s online social networks and motivate them to engage in the health campaign. We use innovative technologies in the field of data analysis to optimize online (health) campaigns without compromising the privacy of young people. We co-create our campaign messages and strategies with young people, parents, and health professionals.
Website